Sex-based differences in drug addiction: stress and epigenetics -- Annie Bryant
Both of week’s papers examined how nature versus nurture can
affect one’s vulnerability to cocaine addiction, with a focus on sex-specific
differences. The latter part is important given that women tend to engage in
cocaine usage younger and more extensively than males, and that maternal versus
paternal cocaine exposure produce differential effect in male and female
offspring. Holly et al. focused on “nurture” with stress as a key factor for
females, while Vassoler et al. explored “nature” with epigenetic reprogramming
of cocaine sensitivity in males.
Holly et al. cite findings that stress increases
vulnerability to drug abuse, implementing an episodic social defeat stress
paradigm in rats. This model was previously shown to increase male response to
cocaine, with elevated behavioral and neural cross-sensitization. I was
surprised that the male and female aggressor rats exhibited equal fight
frequency and duration, and it’s cool that they had qualitatively distinct
fight tactics. Overall, stressed females exhibited greater behavioral
sensitization, neuronal (dopaminergic) sensitization, and cocaine
self-administration than equivalently-stressed males. These differences emerged
at different time points, suggesting different physiological mechanisms by
which stress affects males and females. I wondered if male versus female rats
would have responded differently to different doses of cocaine; perhaps females
have a lower threshold for cocaine sensitivity.
Holly et al. also explored the role of estradiol, since
other studies have highlighted its role in cocaine-induced locomotor and striatal
dopamine sensitization in female rats in estrus. Indeed, in this study,
stressed female rats in estrus did walk longer and more frequently after
cocaine injection than did stressed males or stressed non-estrus females;
however, stressed estrus females didn’t self-administer more cocaine or binge
for longer than non-estrus females. While Holly et al. showed that stressed
females as a whole exhibited greater and longer-lasting DA concentration in the
NAc after cocaine delivery compared to all other groups, it’s unfortunate they
didn’t have enough estrus vs non-estrus female rats in this experiment to have
statistical power. This precluded them from directly comparing DA levels
between these two groups, which (in my opinion) undermines their conclusion
that there is a relationship between stress and estradiol in dopaminergic
function in female rats. Moreover, they didn’t propose any mechanism by which
estradiol might be exerting these effects. It would help to elucidate what
molecules/pathways estradiol interacts with in this setting.
Nevertheless, I do think this paper supports the idea that
stress exerts different effects on drug use proclivity in males versus females.
I like their idea of investigating the causal relationship between estradiol,
DA tone, and cocaine intake by treating ovariectomized female rats with
estradiol and/or progesterone – the latter of which inhibits the effects of
cocaine, as Vassoler et al. point out. Other groups have shown sex-related
differences in dopamine receptor and transporter distribution, which could
influence dopamine reuptake and concentration in the NAc. On that note, I was
hoping for more justification as to why Holly et al. focused on the NAc in
particular, beyond citing findings that cocaine upregulates dopamine expression
there. Also, perhaps this is beyond the scope of this paper, but I would be
interested to see the sex-specific effects of stress alone on locomotion and
dopamine content without cocaine administration in the equation.
Switching gears to focus on nurture, Vassoler et al.
developed a rat model to explore the influence of paternal cocaine intake on
offspring gene expression, chromatin remodeling, and cocaine reinforcement.
This model is advantageous because it eliminates confounding influences of in utero cocaine exposure and doesn’t
influence maternal behavior toward offspring. I thought the sugar cube
experiment was a great control to distinguish reduced acquisition/motivation
from general learning deficits. Male offspring of cocaine-taking sires showed
delayed acquisition of cocaine self-administration in the fixed ratio 1
schedule and reduced motivation for self-administration in the progressive
ratio schedule. In the latter, the difference in breakpoint only emerged at the
higher dose; the authors don’t address this (in fact, they don’t justify using
two different doses at all), and I wonder if this has any relevance to human
cocaine administration.
Vassoler et al. note that cocaine and BDNF have opposing
effects in limbic nuclei: cocaine modulates BDNF expression in limbic nuclei,
which in turn increases cocaine-seeking and intake behaviors. Conversely, BDNF
(in the mPFC, specifically) curbs the effects of cocaine on behavior,
mitigating the intrinsic reinforcing effectiveness of cocaine. They point out
the (bizarre) fact that cocaine concentration in the testes after SC injection
is second only to that in the brain. As such, Vassoler et al. hypothesized that
the male offspring of cocaine-exposed sires inherited this drug-resistance
phenotype via changes in the BDNF gene in the sires’ sperm. They found that
cocaine-exposed sires had increased association between the Bdnf promoter and
transcription-upregulating acetylation of histone H3 in their sperm.
Accordingly, their offspring had increased BDNF mRNA transcripts and protein
levels in the mPFC, with an increased association with acetylated H3.
Furthermore, the BDNF receptor antagonist ANA-12 restored acquisition of
cocaine self-administration in the male cocaine-sired rats, paralleling
previous findings that RNAi-mediated BDNF knockdown in the mPFC increasing
cocaine-seeking PR schedule breakpoint.
I think Vassoler et al. put forth a well-informed and
evidence-supported hypothesis as to how cocaine exposure in male sires affects
male offspring cocaine behavior via BDNF-related chromatin changes in sperm.
However, I’m still unclear as to how these changes would only induce
transcriptional changes and behavioral phenotypes in male offspring. The Bdnf gene is located on chromosome 11,
so it’s not a sex-linked gene. I think it would be interesting to directly
address BDNF levels by comparing controls with either BDNF knock-down or overexpression
and measure resulting behavior from cocaine sires. I also wonder how much of
their findings are specific to cocaine self-administration rather than the
physiological effects of (involuntary) cocaine delivery? For example, how would
offspring behavior compare from sires that were routinely injected with
cocaine? Lastly, I wonder if this model could reproduce findings the authors
cite wherein paternal cocaine intake produces working memory deficits in female
offspring.
Comments
Post a Comment