Han & Yiu: CREB and Memory Allocation -- Annie Bryant
Recalling a particular memory may cause us to think about
memories of closely related events or topics. Is this due to an underlying
structure governing memory organization, in which related memories are
contained by overlapping Hebbian ensembles of neurons? Are neurons that become
incorporated into a memory simply in the right place at the right time, or are
there specific factors that predispose them for memory allocation? In 2007, Han
et al. showed that lateral amygdala (LA) neurons with elevated CREB are
selectively activated by auditory fear memories. They replicated this in 2009 using
a combined model of cre-inducible diphtheria toxin receptors (iDTRs) and a CREB-cre
vector to induce CREB overexpression. After fear memory retrieval, CREB-cre
neurons showed increased activity (i.e. nucleus arc RNA) relative to non-infected neighboring cells. CREB-cre mice
also showed enhanced fear memory formation. Systemic administration of DT after
fear memory formation blocked memory expression for at least 12 days,
suggesting permanent memory erasure. However, these mice were still able to
acquire new fear memories thereafter, indicating that remaining noninfected
neurons picked up the slack.
Han et al. had good controls to demonstrate that the CREB-cre
virus did preferentially infect cells active during the fear conditioning and
that DT didn’t induce any general toxin effects. However, I wasn’t totally sold
on their conclusion that they “establish[ed] a causal link between a defined
subpopulation of [LA] neurons and expression of a fear memory”. This paper is
based entirely on neuronal apoptosis and changes to freezing behavior, yet they
didn’t include an experiment to rule out potential confounding effects on baseline
anxiety or general locomotion, both of which could affect freezing behavior. I
was left with several questions: which of CREB’s myriad cellular roles is
important to memory allocation? In what stage during the fear conditioning
paradigm is CREB expression relevant? Was killing CREB-cre neurons with DT a
better option than simply inhibiting them after memory acquisition?
Yiu et al. pick up where Han et al. left off, noting that
CREB promotes neuronal excitability by (in part) decreasing voltage-gated K+
currents. They hypothesize that this CREB-mediated excitability enhances fear
memory formation and biases engrams to recruit high-CREB neurons. They investigated
this using HSV::CREB and HSV::dnKCNQ2 (a dominant-negative K+ channel), both of
which increased excitability in cultured hippocampal neurons. Interestingly, Kir2.1
(an inwardly rectifying K+ channel) with CREB blocked CREB-induced
excitability. These findings were paralleled in vivo following auditory fear
conditioning: while CREB and dnKCNQ2 injected into principal LA neurons enhanced
memory, CREB+Kir2.1 blocked memory enhancement. Yiu et al. did also account for
potential anxiety and locomotion effects using the elevated plus maze test and
open field test, respectively, and found that their manipulations didn’t affect
either behavioral outcome.
Both Han et al. and Yiu et al. indicated that fear memories
incorporate a large network of neural structures beyond the LA, and Yiu et al. explored
two other amygdala nuclei: basal amygdala (BA) and central amygdala (CeA). When
they injected the CREB vector into BA or CeA, they didn’t observe any fear memory
enhancement associated with CREB overexpression, highlighting the regional
specificity of CREB-enhanced memory. Yu et al. also address the specific time
frame during which neuronal activity is relevant to memory allocation. Viral
delivery of CREB or dnKCNQ2 two days before training (for optimal transgene
expression during training) produced long-lasting memory enhancement beyond the
end of transgene expression. However, viral delivery of CREB or dnKCNQ2 11 days
before or 1 day after training had no effect on fear memory formation or recall.
These series of experiments support the conclusion that neuronal excitability
must be increased at the time of training in order to enhance memory.
To
delve deeper in temporal specificity, they also used DREADDs and optogenetics,
with which they could transiently excite neurons with fine temporal and spatial
control. They used the DREADD hM3Dq, a synthetic ACh receptor that increases
excitability by binding the synthetic ligand CNO. When hM3Dq-injected mice were
systemically administered CNO right before fear training, they exhibited memory
enhancement 24 hours later – in the absence of CNO. Conversely, delivery of CNO
after training failed to enhance memory, further indicating that excitability during
training and memory encoding (rather than during consolidation) is key for
memory enhancement. Furthermore, artificial reactivation of these neurons with
CNO in a novel context test was sufficient to induce freezing. Lastly, Yiu et
al. used ChR2 to show that activation of a small subset of LA neurons immediately
before auditory fear was sufficient to enhance memory formation. These
experiments seemed a bit overkill since they all drove home the same point:
neurons must be excited at the time of memory acquisition to be incorporated into
that memory and strengthen it.
Interestingly, memory traces were quite stable in size
regardless of experimental group; however, neurons expressing CREB, dnKCNQ2, or
hM3Dq were proportionally over-represented among arc+ neurons. Accordingly, noninfected neurons were
under-represented among arc+ neurons, and were even less likely to be activated
by fear training in CREB or dnKCNQ2 mice than in GFP mice. This suggests neurons
compete to become part of a memory based on relative excitability, which echoes
findings in Kim et al. 2013: the competition for fear memory allocation depends
on both neuronal excitability and disynaptic inhibition – in which one
excitatory neuron can inhibit another via an inhibitory interneuron.
Yiu et al. point out that although their manipulations were
artificial, studies across species indicate that learning involves changes in
intrinsic excitability via downregulating AHP K+ currents. Their artificial
reactivation of hM3Dq-expressing neurons via CNO in a novel context reminded me
of the Ramirez papers last week. In both studies, two stimuli (e.g. tone or
light + shock) activated distinct populations of neurons that wouldn’t normally
fire at the same time, and linked these neurons in a memory trace that can be
reactivated with the CS. I wonder if this has implications for psychological
disorders involving inappropriate sensory/memory associations like PTSD and
schizophrenia; do individuals with such disorders have higher levels of CREB in
fear-associated brain regions?
Comments
Post a Comment